Kangaroo mother care and the bonding hypothesis

Kangaroo Mother Care and the Bonding Hypothesis

PEDIATRICS Vol. 102 No. 2 August 1998, p. e17

Réjean Tessier*, Marta Cristo, Stella Velez, Marta Girón, SW; Zita Figueroa de Calume, Juan G. Ruiz-Paláez, Yves Charpak, and Nathalie Charpak

From the *School of Psychology, Laval University, Québec, Canada; ISS-World Lab, Kangaroo Mother Care Program, Clinica del Nino, Santa Fe de Bogotá, Colombia; - Clinical Epidemiology Unit, Faculty of Medicine, Javeriana University, Santa Fe de Bogotá, Colombia; and EVAL (Institut pour l'Évaluation dans le domaine Médical, Médico-social et de Santé Publique), Paris, France.

REFERENCES

  1. Charpak N, Ruiz-Pelaez JG, Figueroa de Calume Z 
    Current knowledge of kangaroo mother intervention. 
    Curr Opin Pediatr. 1996;8:108-112 [Medline]
  2. Doyle LW 
    Kangaroo mother care. 
    Lancet. 1997;350:1721-1722 [Medline]
  3. Charpak N, Ruiz-Pelaez JG, Figueroa de Calume Z, Charpak Y 
    Kangaroo mother intervention versus traditional care. A randomized controlled trial. 
    Pediatrics. 1997;100:682-688 [Abstract/Full Text]
  4. Bergman NJ, Jurisoo LA 
    The "Kangaroo-method" for treating low birthweight babies in developing countries. 
    Trop. Doct. 1994;24:57-80 [Medline]
  5.  Sloan NL, Camacho LW, Rojas EP, Stem C 
    Maternidad leidro Ayora study team: kangaroo mother method: randomized controlled trial of an alternative method of care for stabilized low-birthweight infants. 
    Lancet 1994;344:782-785 [Medline]
  6.  Ludington-Hoe SM, Thompson C, Smith J 
    Kangaroo care: research results, and practice implications and guidelines. 
    Neonatal Network 1994;13:19-26 [Medline]
  7.  Whitelaw A 
    Kangaroo baby care: just a nice experience or an important advance for preterm infants? 
    Pediatrics 1990;55:604-605
  8. Odent M. 
    Birth Reborn. New York, NY: Random House; 1984:86-90
  9.  Christensson K, Siles C, Moreno L, Belaustequi A, De La Fuente P, Lagercrantz H, Puyol P, Winberg J 
    Temperature, metabolic adaptation and crying in healthy full-term newborns cared for skin-to-skin or in a cot. 
    Acta Paediatr 1992;81:488-493 [Medline]
  10.  Legault M, Goulet C 
    Comparison of kangaroo and traditional methods of removing preterm infants from incubators. 
    J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs 1995;24:501-506 [Abstract]
  11.  de Leeuw R, Colin EM, Dunnebier EA, Mirmiran M 
    Physiological effects of kangaroo-care in very small preterm infants. 
    Biol Neonate 1991;11:149-55
  12. Anderson GC 
    Current knowledge about skin-to-skin (kangaroo) care for preterm infants. 
    J Perinatol 1991;11:216-228 [Medline]
  13. Klaus MH, Kennell JH. 
    Maternal-Infant Bonding. St Louis, MO: Mosby; 1976
  14.  Klaus MH, Jerauld R, Kreger NC, 
    Maternal attachment: importance of the first post-partum days. 
    N Engl J Med 1972;286:460-463 [Medline]
  15. Lamb ME, Hwang CP. Maternal attachment and mother-neonate bonding: a critical review. In: Lamb ME, Brown AL, eds. 
    Advances in Developmental Psychology. Vol 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1982:1-39
  16. Eyer DE. 
    Mother-Infant Bonding: A Scientific Fiction. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1992
  17. Peterson GH, Mehl LE 
    Some determinants of maternal attachment. 
    Am J Psychiatry. 1978;135:1168-1173
  18.  Rose J, Boggs T Jr, Rosenberg I, Lind J 
    The evidence for a syndrome of "mothering disability" consequent to threats to the survival of neonates: a design for hypothesis testing including prevention in a prospective study. 
    Am J Dis Child. 1960;100:776-786
  19. Meinert CL, Tonascia S. 
    Clinical Trials Design. Conduct and Analysis. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1986;208-216
  20. Barnard KE, Hammond MA, Booth CL, Bee HL, Mitchel SK, Spieker SJ. 
    Measurement and meaning of parent-child interaction. In: Morrison FJ, Lord C, Keating DP, eds. 
    Applied Developmental Psychology. Vol 3. 
    Psychological Development in Infancy. New York, NY: Academic Press Inc; 1989:40-81
  21. Affonso D, Bosque E, Wahlberg V, Brady J 
    Reconciliation and healing for mothers through skin-to-skin contact provided in an American tertiary level intensive care nursery. 
    Neonatal Network 1993;12:25-32
  22. Klaus MH, Kennell JH. 
    Parent-Infant Bonding. St Louis, MO: Mosby; 1982
  23. Landry SH, Chapieski ML, Schmidt M 
    Effects of maternal attention-directing strategies on preterms’ response to toys. 
    Infant Behav Dev 1986;9:257-269
  24. Crnic KA, Ragozin AS, Greenberg MT, Robinson NM, Basham RB 
    Social interaction and developmental competence of preterm and full-term infants during the first year of life. 
    Child Dev. 1983;54:1199-1210 [Medline]
  25. World Health Organization (WHO). 
    Kangaroo Mother Intervention. Presented at a conference supported by The Collaborating Center for Maternal and Child Health; July 1996; Trieste, Italy

Pediatrics (ISSN 0031 4005). © 1998 by the American Academy of Pediatrics

Kangaroo Mother Care and the Bonding Hypothesis

PEDIATRICS Vol. 102 No. 2 August 1998, p. e17

Réjean Tessier*, Marta Cristo, Stella Velez, Marta Girón, SW; Zita Figueroa de Calume, Juan G. Ruiz-Paláez, Yves Charpak, and Nathalie Charpak

From the *School of Psychology, Laval University, Québec, Canada; ISS-World Lab, Kangaroo Mother Care Program, Clinica del Nino, Santa Fe de Bogotá, Colombia; - Clinical Epidemiology Unit, Faculty of Medicine, Javeriana University, Santa Fe de Bogotá, Colombia; and EVAL (Institut pour l'Évaluation dans le domaine Médical, Médico-social et de Santé Publique), Paris, France.

FOOTNOTES

Received for publication Jun 24, 1997; accepted Mar 30, 1998.

Address correspondence to 
Réjean Tessier, PhD, 
School of Psychology, Université Laval, 
Ste-Foy, Québec, 
Canada, G1K 7P4.


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This study was supported jointly by grants from COLCIENCIAS (Colombian government), the World Laboratory (nongovernmental organization, Lausanne, Switzerland, project MCD 13), the ISS (Instituto de Seguros Sociales), Colombia, and UNICEF.

We thank all members of the Kangaroo research team of the Clinica del Nino: A. Mondragón, MD; R. Gómez, MD; R. Martínez, RN; F.A. Gómez, RN; and Mrs M.V. Jiménez for facilitating the many steps of the process. Without their commitment and devotion, this study could not have been developed. We also thank Ms Pascale Mercier for codifying part of the mother-infant interactions, and Ms. Line Nadeau for help with data processing and statistical analyses. We acknowledge general practitioners and neonatal intensive care unit and nurses at the San Pedro Claver Hospital for their support throughout the project. Finally, we thank the parents for their cooperation and participation.


ABBREVIATIONS

KMC
kangaroo mother care
LBW
low birth weight
NICU
neonatal intensive care unit.

Pediatrics (ISSN 0031 4005). © 1998 by the American Academy of Pediatrics

Kangaroo Mother Care and the Bonding Hypothesis

PEDIATRICS Vol. 102 No. 2 August 1998, p. e17

Réjean Tessier*, Marta Cristo, Stella Velez, Marta Girón, SW; Zita Figueroa de Calume, Juan G. Ruiz-Paláez, Yves Charpak, and Nathalie Charpak

From the *School of Psychology, Laval University, Québec, Canada; ISS-World Lab, Kangaroo Mother Care Program, Clinica del Nino, Santa Fe de Bogotá, Colombia; - Clinical Epidemiology Unit, Faculty of Medicine, Javeriana University, Santa Fe de Bogotá, Colombia; and EVAL (Institut pour l'Évaluation dans le domaine Médical, Médico-social et de Santé Publique), Paris, France.

DISCUSSION

The psychological impact of KMC is obvious, but it also is more complex than we had initially thought. The mothers in the KMC group who carried their infant in the skin-to-skin position felt more competent than did their TC counterparts. The infant’s health status, however, was also a major determinant of the mothers’ attachment behavioral patterns.

The Subjective Bonding Indices

The mothers’ global perception of giving birth to a premature infant was different in the two groups. Mothers in the KMC group felt more competent, but also more isolated than did mothers in the TC control groups. First, the sense of competence was clearly much higher in the KMC group, and especially when the intervention started soon after birth (1 to 2 days). In this subgroup, the infants were basically healthy at birth and had had an early close contact with their mothers. Is there is a skin-to-skin effect? Because we had an a priori control on many variables, we suggest that the difference observed between these two groups may be attributable to close contact between the mother and child. It might be explained by empowerment of the mother’s feelings, making her more responsible and confident in her capacity to care for her infant. In contradiction to Whitelaw and colleagues,7 but according to Legault10 and Affonso and coworkers,21 we conclude that skin-to-skin contact at discharge is more effective in terms of the mothers’ feelings than is traditional routine care in hospitals. Moreover, because the difference between the two groups decreases gradually as the delay between birth and start of TC or KMC intervention increased (Table 4), we can corroborate that early timing is more effective than late timing as a means to enhance the mothers’ sense of competence toward her premature infant.

The timing (delay) of contact between a mother and her child after birth has been an important theme in studies aimed at challenging the bonding hypothesis. In Klaus and colleagues’ first study,14 the mothers in the experimental group had 1 hour of skin-to-skin mother-infant contact (within the first 3 hours), followed by 5 hours’ contact per day for 3 successive days. The authors concluded that the increased contact had enhanced the mothers’ attachment behavioral patterns, but they could not determine whether it was the initial 1-hour contact or the 15 hours of additional contact that produced the effect. Because the idea of a very short parent-infant bonding period has been widely criticized, Klaus and Kennell in 198222 extended it to several hours or days after birth. However, we still lack the empirical data to determine the optimal length of time required -- in these first few hours or days after birth -- to produce an effect. From the data obtained in this study, we can empirically suggest that close mother-infant contact during the first 2 days after birth is optimal to produce a major change in a mother’s sense of competence toward her infant.

Furthermore, based on our data, we also can extend the latter finding to infants that have a poor to bad health status after birth. We found that in infants who needed intensive care, the mothers had a heightened feeling of competence in the KMC group relative to those in the TC group. This finding was especially true for the subgroup that left the hospital earlier, ie, at between 3 and 14 days (data not shown). This discovery indicates a definite advantage of using skin-to-skin contact as early as possible, suggesting that kangaroo-carrying interventions should be encouraged during the NICU period. We are thus tempted to speculate that skin-to-skin contact is not only beneficial in the first days but also at any time during the perinatal period.

Second, the KMC intervention also produced negative feelings in the mothers. They felt more isolated than did mothers in the TC group. This was especially true for those whose infant spent a longer time in hospital. This may have occurred when the infant could not gain sufficient weight or suckle properly, had an infectious disease, or was sick in any way. These mothers may feel burdened with too many responsibilities in taking care of the infant and, consequently, feel overwhelmed and that they are not getting sufficient help from the hospital staff and family. This suggests that in such cases, we should add a social support to the KMC’s usual components.

However, feelings of stress and worry in the mothers in the KMC group were maintained at a mean level in any Delay condition, which was not the case for the mothers in the TC group whose stress level was delay-dependent. The latter felt less preoccupied than did mothers in the KMC group when the infant left the hospital early, but they felt much more stressed when the infant stayed longer. Our hypothesis is that KMC gives the mother a feeling of control over her stress and worry about the infant’s health, and that this sensation acts in a protective manner, making her more stress-resilient.

We thus confirm the first part of our hypotheses related to the mothers’ perceptions of a premature birth experience. There is a direct intervention effect favoring the KMC mothers’ sense of competence and the TC mothers’ perception of social support, but the moderating effects are more prominent. Mothers in the KMC group had a higher sense competence when the delay was shorter (bonding effect) and when the infants needed intensive care (resilience effect). Feelings of stress for mothers in the KMC group was lower than that for mothers in the TC group when the delay was longer (resilience effect). Finally, mothers in the KMC group felt less supported or more isolated when the delay was longer (isolation effect).

Maternal sensitivity was moderately induced by the KMC intervention in the sense that these mothers were more sensitive and stimulated their infant more cognitively in the context of a longer hospital stay. This may be interpreted as a resilience effect that also was expressed by the mothers’ perceptions of their competence and low stress level in these high-risk situations. However, in the context of the infant needing intensive care, observed maternal sensitivity was not increased directly by skin-to-skin contacts. The infant’s health appeared as a far more important factor, and mothers tended to provide more stimulation to infants who had a poor health status, which, in turn, led to a more responsive infant. Our initial interpretation of this finding was that poor health may increase the mother’s attention, worry, and responsiveness to her infant and that a infant who had been overstimulated and stressed in an NICU -- in some cases for a considerably long time (up to 50 days in this sample) -- might have become oversensitive to any cues, including maternal cues. This interpretation, however, is not confirmed by our data, which showed that the subgroup that spent an average of 17 days in the NICU had a lower responsiveness to their mothers than did infants in the other group who spent an average of 4.4 days in the NICU (data not shown). Therefore, we hypothesize that this marked orientation toward the sick child might be the beginning of a continuing protective behavioral pattern observed frequently in the interaction between a mother and a sick child during the first years of life.23,24 This might be interpreted as a natural trend observed in the mothers’ behaviors aimed at protecting sick and feeble infants, and are as such, well adapted to the child’s situation. However, it could be readily construed that mothers who continue to demonstrate oversensitivity to a child would later be overprotective. This conclusion, however, would require validation in a longitudinal study. At present, we can only conclude that the mothers in both groups, but more consistently in the KMC group, showed behavioral patterns that were adapted to a child’s health status. They were more sensitive and more responsive to weak children. This effect overshadowed the KMC carrying effect.

In conclusion, observations of the mothers’ sensitive behavior did not show a definite bonding effect, but rather a resilience effect. This is attributable to the KMC intervention: mothers in the KMC group were more sensitive toward an at-risk infant whose development has been threatened by a longer hospital stay. Otherwise, we observed that mothers in both the KMC group and the TC group had behavioral patterns that were adapted to the child’s at-risk health status and to the precarious condition of some premature infants requiring intensive care. We conclude that the infant’s health status may be a more prominent factor in explaining a mother’s more sensitive behavior, which overshadowed the kangaroo carrying effect in our study.

From a subjective perspective, results are different. We observed a change in the mothers’ perception of her child, which was attributable to the skin-to-skin contact in the kangaroo carrying position. This effect was related to a subjective bonding effect that may be understood readily by the empowering nature of the KMC intervention. Moreover, in particular situations when the infant needs intensive care at birth or has to remain in hospital longer, mothers who carried their infant in the kangaroo position felt more competent than did mothers in the TC control group. This is what we call a resilience effect. There also was an apparent negative effect on the KMC mothers’ feelings: when the infant has to stay in hospital longer because of health problems or gestational immaturity, there appeared to be a gap between the mothers’ stronger needs to be helped and the feeling of received support. We interpret this as an isolation effect. To minimize this deleterious effect, we would suggest adding social support as an integral component of KMC.

These results suggest that KMC should be promoted actively and that mothers should be encouraged to use it as soon as possible during the intensive care period, up to 40 weeks’ gestational age. Thus, KMC should be viewed as a means of humanizing the process of giving birth in a context of prematurity. This finding confirms the conclusions of the 1996 Trieste workshop25 suggesting that KMC should be promoted both in hospitals and after early discharge.


Pediatrics (ISSN 0031 4005). © 1998 by the American Academy of Pediatrics

Kangaroo Mother Care and the Bonding Hypothesis

PEDIATRICS Vol. 102 No. 2 August 1998, p. e17

Réjean Tessier*, Marta Cristo, Stella Velez, Marta Girón, SW; Zita Figueroa de Calume, Juan G. Ruiz-Paláez, Yves Charpak, and Nathalie Charpak

From the *School of Psychology, Laval University, Québec, Canada; ISS-World Lab, Kangaroo Mother Care Program, Clinica del Nino, Santa Fe de Bogotá, Colombia; - Clinical Epidemiology Unit, Faculty of Medicine, Javeriana University, Santa Fe de Bogotá, Colombia; and EVAL (Institut pour l'Évaluation dans le domaine Médical, Médico-social et de Santé Publique), Paris, France.

RESULTS

The data presented in Tables 1 and 2 show that the KMC group and the TC group were identical at the conceptional age of 41 weeks based on sociodemographic criteria and factors related to pregnancy and labor. A significant difference both in the gestational age and in the infant’s weight at birth and at eligibility was observed that had completely disappeared at the time of observations (41 weeks; Table 2). Because weight at eligibility is the most representative group difference before start of intervention, it will be used as covariate in the analyses.


TABLE 1

A Comparison Between the KMC Group and the TC Control Group Based on Sociodemographic, Labor, and Delivery Characteristics

View this table


TABLE 2

A Comparison Between the KMC Group and the TC Control Group Based on Factors Related to Newborn Infants

View this table


Two-way analysis of variance stratifying by birth weight showed that the savings in hospital stays were clearly related to weight at birth: an interaction effect (F(3480) = 4.06, P < .01) shows that the maximum saving in the KMC group was observed in infants weighing <1501 g (4.5 to 6.7 days), whereas in infants weighing >1500 g, the length of hospital stay was virtually identical in both groups (Table 3). As expected, there was no group effect in the NICU length of stay (F(1480) = 1.79, NS), which, however, increased with a decrease in birth weight (F(3480) = 22.6, P < .001).


TABLE 3

Differences in Hospital Stay and Need of NICU Patterns from Birth to Term by Intervention Groups (KMC vs TC) and Birth Weight Categories

View this table


Although the length of intervention (expressed by the kangaroo position in the KMC group and by time between eligibility and 41 weeks’ conceptional age in the TC group) was not related to dependent variables, it has not been used in additional analyses. Data analyses were then performed with two moderating conditions. The first was the interval between birth and eligibility, representing the period during which the mother was separated from the infant before beginning the intervention. It has been trichotomized: 1 to 2 days is the first category, including infants born in fairly good health and randomized shortly after birth. In this subgroup, infants left the hospital with their mothers and received either KMC or TC at home. A 3- to 14-day delay makes up the second category, and >14-day delay makes up the third category, representing a long separation before closer mother-infant contact.

The second moderating condition is the child’s health, measured by the duration of stay in the NICU. It is dichotomized as "yes" or "no." This second moderating variable is statistically independent of the delay between birth and eligibility (first moderating variable). Dependent variables are twofold: the first is the mother’s perception of the experience of a premature birth, and the second is the mother and child’s sensitivity to each other in a feeding situation. All statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS 7.5 for Windows.


KMC Benefits and the Timing of Intervention

The first set of analyses (multivariate analyses of variance) was computed using Groups as the independent condition and Delay Before Intervention as the moderating variable. These analyses were completed alternating with the mothers’ perception and their sensitivity as dependent variables (Table 4). The hypothesis suggests an interaction effect for length of delay and Group. Based on the Mother’s Perception of Premature Birth Questionnaire (perceptions scores) data for the 24-hour postnatal interview, no differences between the KMC group and the TC group were found - mothers in both groups reported the same general feelings about their recent experience. However, based on the 41-week (conceptional age) interview, there were some group differences (F(3479) = 5.33,P = .001). Sense of competence was particularly higher for mothers in the KMC group (F(1481) = 10.36, P = .001), and social support was perceived as lower for mothers in the KMC than for those in the TC group (F(1481) = 5.03, P = .03). No delay effect was found. However, in the stress and worry subscale, the data show an interaction effect: the longer the separation, the more stressed were mothers in the TC group (F(2481) = 3.07, P = .05). No covariate (infant’s weight at eligibility) effect was observed. The data also suggest the following: 1) The mother’s sense of competence was higher in the KMC group, regardless of timing of the intervention. However, post hoc analyses indicated that kangaroo-carrying practiced earlier (1 or 2 days) after birth modified the mothers’ sense of competence to a greater degree. 2) Mothers in the TC group felt more supported than did their KMC counterparts, and post hoc comparisons confirmed that this is particularly true when the infant remained in hospital longer. Finally, 3) the TC mothers’ feelings of stress increased with the time their infant spent in hospital, which was not the case for mothers in the KMC group.


TABLE 4

Mother’s Perception and Mother and Child’s Observed Sensitivity by Intervention Groups (KMC vs TC) and Delay From Starting Intervention

View this table


Using observational data as dependent variables have shown that mother’s sensitivity was higher in the KMC group (F(1481)= 3.71, P = .05). Interaction effects on the duration of the infant’s hospital stay before starting the KMC or TC intervention (Delay) also were found (Table 4). A Delay × Group effect was observed with respect to the mother’s sensitivity (F(2481) = 4.23, P = .02) and the mother’s cognitive stimulations (F(2481) = 3.18, P = .04), whereas mothers in the KMC group were more sensitive to the infant staying in hospital longer (>14 days) compared with mothers in the TC group. Finally, a slight but significant delay difference was observed, where the "3- to 14-day delay" subgroup of mothers responded more adequately to the child’s distress than did those in the other subgroups (F(2481) = 3.04, P = .05). These results show that the delay before starting the KMC or the TC intervention affects only marginally the expression of maternal sensitivity toward her infant. The type of intervention (KMC vs TC) is more likely to influence this maternal behavior, especially with longer infant hospital stay.


KMC Benefits and Infant’s Stay in the NICU Before Intervention

The second set of analyses uses the NICU as a moderating variable. This is an indicator of infant’s health status between birth and eligibility. This series of analyses aims to show the moderating effect of the infant’s relative weakness on the mothers’ perceptions and sensitivity toward her child. According to the first set of analyses, data revealed a strong Group effect, where mothers in the KMC group showed a greater sense of competence than mothers in the TC group (F(1483) = 11.02, P = .001; Table 5). However post hoc analyses reveal that the mothers’ sense of competence is modified to a greater degree when infants needed intensive care after birth. Marginally, a trend was seen in mothers whose infant needed intensive care to feel more socially supported (F(1483) = 2.61, P = .11). No interaction or covariate effect was observed.


TABLE 5

Mother’s Perception and Mother and Child’s Observed Sensitivity by Intervention Groups (KMC vs TC) and Child’s Need of NICU at Birth

View this table


The mothers’ sensitivity scores were not related directly to early skin-to-skin contact, but rather to the infant’s health during the inpatient period. The mothers sensitivity was generally higher when the infant had spent some time in the NICU compared with the dyads who had not (F(6478) = 2,21, P = .04). In particular, we found that in the former case, the mothers were more sensitive (F(1483) = 6.41, P = .01) and stimulated the infant more cognitively (F(1483) = 7.84, P = .005) and socioemotively (F(1483) = 8.14, P = .005), and the infants gave clearer cues (F(1483) = 5.42, P = .02) and were more responsive to their mother (F(1483) = 3.97, P = .05). Consequently, we can say that mother and child’s sensitivity to each other at 41 weeks’ gestational age was higher if the infant needed intensive care at birth. Moreover, interaction effects occur whereby the infant’s weakness affected the mothers in the TC group who stimulated more cognitively (F(1483) = 4.22, P = .04) and socioemotively (F(1483) = 4.28, P = .04) the infants that needed intensive care at birth compared with the mothers in the KMC group.


Pediatrics (ISSN 0031 4005). © 1998 by the American Academy of Pediatrics

Subcategories